Chimpanzees versus humans: it's not that simple.
نویسندگان
چکیده
Povinelli and Vonk [1] argue that human beings have a theory of mind and chimpanzees do not. But this black and white picture is exceedingly misleading. We can add the necessary color – on the basis of reliable scientific findings using a variety of experimental methods – by recognizing that the generic label ‘theory of mind’ actually covers a wide range of processes of social cognition. As we documented in our article in the same issue [2], although chimpanzees almost certainly do not understand other minds in the same way that humans do (e.g. they apparently do not understand beliefs) they do understand some psychological processes (e.g. seeing). There is nothing odd about this. Human children do not understand such things as beliefs until about four years of age. But between one and two years of age they clearly show, in a number of different experimental paradigms, that they understand such psychological processes as seeing, attending, desiring and intending. So do two-year-olds have a theory of mind? It is not a helpful question. The challenge in the study of both comparative and developmental social cognition is to go beyond global characterizations and attempt to specify in detail how organisms understand the psychological processes of other organisms. Povinelli and Vonk have adopted a kind of derived behaviorism, that is, they note that organisms must observe behavior in order to attribute to it underlying psychological processes, and so it is always possible that the organism is simply making ‘behavioral abstractions’. With regard to one of our food-competition studies in particular [3], Povinelli and Vonk point out some possible behavioral cues that the subordinate chimpanzee might have used to determinewhether the dominant chimpanzee had seen food over which the two of them were competing (and use this information to decide whether to pursue that food). These are such things as: ‘avoid food when a specific individual has previously oriented to it in its current location (and not in another location)’. How chimpanzees might have acquired such rules is not specified, nor is there any speculation about how they might understand them. But our most serious objection is that Povinelli and Vonk ignore the control conditions in our first study in this series [4]. In one of those conditions, the food was hidden and the subordinate made its choice while the dominant’s door was down: thus, there was no behavior to read. And still the subordinates went for the food that the dominant would not be able to see when her door was opened. In another condition, we put the food on the dominant’s side of the barrier and allowed the subordinate to observe her behavior for some moments. When released, subordinates had no clue where the food was: that is, when given the chance to read behavior, they did not. Some ad hoc explanation in terms of behavior and context reading is undoubtedly possible in these cases as well, and indeed Povinelli and Vonk despair that we will never be able to rule out all possible explanations based on behavioral cues in our experiments (indeed, this is probably also true when adult human beings are subjects; and Methodological Behaviorism institutionalizes this despair). Povinelli and Vonk’s proposed solution is fine, and we agree with it, but it is nothing new. They propose experiments in which subjects are exposed to novel situations so as to rule out past learning experience as a potential explanation, but that is precisely what we, and many others, have been trying to do. For example, in one of our experimental conditions [3] we placed food behind a transparent barrier, of which our subjects had had no previous experience, to our knowledge. The subordinates were not fooled; they knew that it did not obstruct the dominant’s visual access to the food. Perhaps this situation is not novel enough for Povinelli and Vonk, but in our opinion the novelty ought to stay within the bounds of principles of ecological validity. Thus, the specific experiment that Povinelli and Vonk propose as a preferable alternative to ours (interacting with buckets placed over their heads) has very low ecological validity. In fact, it has already been done with human children, and they fail until school age [5] (which must mean that four-year-olds do not have a theory of mind after all...) Of course it is possible that human beings are the only species that understand any psychological processes in others, and we ourselves held this position not so very long Corresponding author: Michael Tomasello ([email protected]). Update TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.6 June 2003 239
منابع مشابه
Children, but Not Chimpanzees, Prefer to Collaborate
Human societies are built on collaborative activities. Already from early childhood, human children are skillful and proficient collaborators. They recognize when they need help in solving a problem and actively recruit collaborators [1, 2]. The societies of other primates are also to some degree cooperative. Chimpanzees, for example, engage in a variety of cooperative activities such as border...
متن کاملComparative nuclear and mitochondrial genome diversity in humans and chimpanzees.
Restriction mapping and sequencing have shown that humans have substantially lower levels of mitochondrial genome diversity (d) than chimpanzees. In contrast, humans have substantially higher levels of heterozygosity (H) at protein-coding loci, suggesting a higher level of diversity in the nuclear genome. To investigate the discrepancy further, we sequenced a segment of the mitochondrial genome...
متن کاملReferential gestural communication in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
with Bill Frost that was begun in Getting's lab. I also have been very interested in the evolution of neural circuits. As a graduate student, I was influenced by the work of Dorothy Paul and Edmund Arbus. I think that understanding a neural circuit means also understanding its evolutionary history. It's a mistake to try to analyze a circuit as if it were 'created' for its current function. Spea...
متن کاملDifferences and similarities between human and chimpanzee neural progenitors during cerebral cortex development
Human neocortex expansion likely contributed to the remarkable cognitive abilities of humans. This expansion is thought to primarily reflect differences in proliferation versus differentiation of neural progenitors during cortical development. Here, we have searched for such differences by analysing cerebral organoids from human and chimpanzees using immunohistofluorescence, live imaging, and s...
متن کاملDoubts about complex speciation between humans and chimpanzees.
Two patterns from large-scale DNA sequence data have been put forward as evidence that speciation between humans and chimpanzees was complex, involving hybridization and strong selection. First, divergence between humans and chimpanzees varies considerably across the autosomes. Second, divergence between humans and chimpanzees (but not gorillas) is markedly lower on the X chromosome. Here, we d...
متن کاملA human reproductive approach to the study of infertility in chimpanzees: An experience at Leon’s Zoological Park, Mexico
Great apes are mammals close to humans in their genetic, behavioral, social and evolutionary characteristics and new genomic information is revolutionizing our understanding of evolution in primates. However, all these species are endangered. While there are many global programs to protect these species, the International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) projects that in a near future th...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید
ثبت ناماگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید
ورودعنوان ژورنال:
- Trends in cognitive sciences
دوره 7 6 شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2003